SILC Showcase

Showcase February 2011: Finding the middle: Spatial language facilitates spatial reasoning

Share this article using our bitly.com url: http://bit.ly/1rXaNe0

Finding the middle: Spatial language facilitates spatial reasoning   Open .pdf document

Nina Simms, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University
Dedre Gentner (Co-PI), Department of Psychology, Northwestern University
David Uttal, Department of Psychology, Northwestern University

Spatial relations are fundamental to spatial thinking. Understanding concepts like left and right, for example, facilitate navigation. Parallel and perpendicular are spatial relations that are unavoidable in geometry and engineering. However, relational concepts can be difficult for young children (Gentner & Rattermann, 1991), and some relations are more challenging than others. For example, young children have difficulty with the spatial relation midpoint (i.e., middle) (MacDonald, Spetch, Kelly, & Cheng, 2004; Spetch & Parent, 2006; but see Uttal, Sandstrom, & Newcombe, 2006). Understanding midpoint is important for other science and math concepts like balance, half, and bisect, to name a few. Compared to some of the spatial relations that young children understand, like on, in, or under, midpoint is a complex relation. It encodes location relative to more than one entity (e.g., in the middle of the sofa and the chair). Midpoint is also unusual because it integrates both qualitative (i.e., between) and quantitative spatial information (e.g., three inches from both entities).

When do children begin to understand the midpoint relation, and what helps them do so? A previous study on midpoint found that on a challenging midpoint search task, the only children who succeeded were those who also spontaneously described the location as “in the middle” (MacDonald et al., 2004). This is consistent with other research showing that spatial language is related to and facilitates spatial reasoning (Dessalegn & Landau, 2008; Hermer-Vazquez, Moffet, & Munkholm, 2001; Loewenstein & Gentner, 2005; Pruden, Levine, & Huttenlocher, 2010). Thus, in a series of studies we explore the relationship between spatial language and midpoint understanding. In all studies, we use a Midpoint Search Task to evaluate children's understanding of the midpoint relation. The task involves hiding a small treasure box for children to find. The treasure box is always hidden exactly in the middle of two flags, and the flags may be placed in different locations and at different distances from each other (see Figure 1).

Figure 1

Figure 1. Midpoint Search Task Apparatus

In our first study (Simms & Gentner, 2008), we administered the Midpoint Search Task to 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds. We also assessed children's comprehension and production of the words middle and between, along with some other spatial terms. In general, children's ability to encode midpoint improved with age, F(2,59) = 13.27, p < .001, especially between 3- and 4-years-old, p < .001. (Figure 2). Even more striking was that children’s knowledge of the words middle and between predicted their success on the search task, beyond the improvements seen with age, ß = 0.27, p < .05. Also, as MacDonald and colleagues (2004) found, children who produced the words middle or between during the task outperformed children who did not (Figure 3).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Children’s accuracy on Midpoint Search Task. Children's performance improved with age.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Children’s accuracy on Midpoint Search Task. Children who produced words middle or between outperformed children who did not.

In our second study, we asked whether we could improve the youngest children's performance on the Midpoint Search Task by using the word middle during the task. For a new group of 3-year-olds, when children found the treasure box on each trial, the experimenter told them that they had found it, “right in the middle of the two flags”. The use of the word middle during the task significantly improved children's performance on the challenging search task, F(1,44) = 4.12, p < .05. (Figure 4).

Figure 4

Figure 4. Children’s accuracy on Midpoint Search Task. Hearing the word middle improved children’s performance.

In two ongoing studies, we are further exploring how relational language can benefit young children's reasoning about the midpoint relation. If, as our first study suggests, the ability to label a spatial relation enables children to better reason about that relation (recall that children who produced the words middle or between were more successful on the search task), then teaching children a label for the midpoint (i.e., middle) ahead of time should also facilitate their performance. Thus, in one ongoing study, we are teaching children about the midpoint relation, with or without using the label middle, to see whether children who have learned the label are better able to encode the midpoint on the Midpoint Search Task. Preliminary results suggest that children are indeed more successful if they have learned the label middle.

In a second ongoing study, we compare the efficacy of labels to another tool that has been shown to help children reason about spatial relations: maps (Uttal, Fisher, & Taylor, 2006). Maps differ from labels in important ways (Davies & Uttal, 2007). Labels convey spatial information sequentially, map arbitrarily to spatial concepts, and usually represent only qualitative information. In contrast, maps convey information about multiple spatial relations simultaneously, map fairly veridically to spatial relations, and represent both qualitative and quantitative information. Accordingly, maps could be a particularly effective way to communicate the unique and challenging aspects of midpoint. Thus, the current study explores the relative effectiveness of labels and maps, separately and in combination, as tools to communicate spatial relational information to preschoolers. Preliminary results suggest, consistent with prior findings, that hearing a label during the search task improves their performance, even after the label is no longer used. Surprisingly, however, the results so far do not show a benefit for seeing a map. Though children this age have been shown to successfully use maps (e.g., Bluestein & Acredolo, 1979), they may not be well-suited to communicate spatial information for this task at these ages.

These studies add to a growing literature demonstrating the power of language as a tool to enhance children's spatial reasoning. Future research should begin to address the conditions under which spatial language are most beneficial and translate this research into methods that can be used by parents and educators to help children become effective spatial reasoners.

References

  • ♦ Bluestein, N. & Acredolo, L. P. (1979). Developmental changes in map- reading skills. Child Development, 50, 691-697.
  • ♦ Davies, C. & Uttal, D. H. (2007). Map use and the development of spatial cognition. In J. Plumert & J. Spencer (Eds.), The Emerging Spatial Mind (pp. 219-247). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • ♦ Dessalegn, B. & Landau, B. (2008). More than meets the eye: The role of language in binding visual properties. Psychological Science, 19 (2), 189-195.
  • ♦ Gentner, D. & Rattermann, M. J. (1991). Language and the career of similarity. In S. A. Gelman & J. P. Brynes (Eds.), Perspectives on thought and language: Interrelations in development (pp. 225-277). London: Cambridge University Press.
  • ♦ Hermer-Vazquez, L., Moffet, A. & Munkholm, P. (2001). Language, space, and the development of cognitive ?exibility in humans: The case of two spatial memory tasks. Cognition, 78, 263–299
  • ♦ Loewenstein, J. & Gentner, D. (2005). Relational language and the development of relational mapping. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 315-353.
  • ♦ MacDonald, S. E., Spetch, M. L., Kelly, D. M. & Cheng, K. (2004). Strategies in landmark use by children, adults, and marmoset monkeys. Learning and Motivation, 35, 322-347.
  • ♦ Pruden, S.M., Levine, S.C. & Huttenlocher, J. (2010). Individual differences in children's spatial language use predicts later spatial cognition. Paper presented in S. Roseberry & T. Göksun,(Chairs), When representational systems collide: Aligning space and language. International Society on Infant Studies, Baltimore, MD (March, 2010).
  • ♦ Simms, N. & Gentner, D. (2008). Spatial language and landmark use: Can 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds find the middle? In Sloutsky, V., Love, B. & McRae, K. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society.
  • ♦ Spetch, M. L. & Parent, M. B. (2006). Age and sex differences in children's spatial search strategies. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 807-812.
  • ♦ Uttal, D. H., Fisher, J. A. & Taylor, H. A. (2006). Words and maps: Children's mental models of spatial information acquired from maps and from descriptions. Developmental Science, 9(2), 221-235.
  • ♦ Uttal, D. H., Sandstrom, L. B. & Newcombe, N. S. (2006). One hidden object, two spatial codes: Young children's use of relational and vector coding. Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 503-525.
You are here: SILC Home Page SILC Showcase Showcase February 2011: Finding the middle: Spatial language facilitates spatial reasoning